Cognitive Function: A Comprehensive Review of Neural Mechanisms, Influential Factors, and Plasticity

Cognitive Function: A Comprehensive Review of Neural Mechanisms, Influential Factors, and Plasticity

Abstract

Cognitive function encompasses a broad spectrum of mental processes that enable individuals to perceive, learn, reason, remember, and interact effectively with their environment. This research report provides a comprehensive overview of cognitive function, exploring its underlying neural mechanisms, the multifaceted factors that influence its development and decline, and the remarkable capacity of the brain to adapt and reorganize in response to experience and injury – a concept known as cognitive plasticity. We delve into specific cognitive domains, including attention, memory, language, executive function, and visuospatial abilities, examining their neural substrates and interdependencies. Furthermore, we analyze the impact of various factors, such as genetics, age, lifestyle choices (e.g., diet, exercise, substance use), and neurological or psychiatric conditions, on cognitive performance. The report also addresses the potential for cognitive enhancement through interventions targeting neural plasticity, including cognitive training, pharmacological approaches, and lifestyle modifications. Finally, we identify critical gaps in current research and propose future directions for advancing our understanding of cognitive function and developing effective strategies to promote cognitive health and resilience throughout the lifespan.

Many thanks to our sponsor Maggie who helped us prepare this research report.

1. Introduction

Cognitive function is the cornerstone of human intelligence and adaptive behavior. It allows us to navigate the complexities of the world, solve problems, make decisions, and form meaningful relationships. Understanding the intricacies of cognitive function is crucial not only for advancing basic neuroscience but also for developing effective interventions to address cognitive impairments associated with aging, neurological disorders, and psychiatric conditions. The study of cognition has evolved from philosophical inquiries to a multidisciplinary field integrating neuroscience, psychology, computer science, and genetics.

This report aims to provide a comprehensive overview of cognitive function, focusing on the following key areas: (1) the neural mechanisms underlying different cognitive domains; (2) the factors that influence cognitive development, stability, and decline; (3) the concept of cognitive plasticity and its role in adaptation and recovery; and (4) the potential for cognitive enhancement through targeted interventions. We will critically evaluate existing research, highlighting areas of consensus and ongoing debate, and identify promising avenues for future investigation.

Many thanks to our sponsor Maggie who helped us prepare this research report.

2. Neural Substrates of Cognitive Function

The human brain is a complex and highly interconnected network, and different brain regions contribute to specific cognitive functions. While some cognitive processes are localized to particular areas, most cognitive tasks involve the coordinated activity of multiple brain regions, forming distributed neural networks.

2.1 Attention

Attention is a fundamental cognitive process that allows us to selectively focus on relevant information while ignoring distractions. Several brain regions are critical for attention, including the prefrontal cortex (PFC), parietal cortex, and the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC). The PFC is involved in sustained attention, working memory, and goal-directed behavior [1]. The parietal cortex, particularly the intraparietal sulcus, plays a role in spatial attention and orienting to stimuli [2]. The ACC is involved in monitoring conflict and regulating attention to resolve interference [3].

The dorsal and ventral attention networks, which operate in a complementary fashion, further describe the neurological underpinnings. The dorsal attention network, including the intraparietal sulcus (IPS) and frontal eye fields (FEF), is involved in top-down, goal-directed attention, while the ventral attention network, which includes the temporoparietal junction (TPJ) and ventral frontal cortex (VFC), mediates bottom-up, stimulus-driven attention [4].

2.2 Memory

Memory is the ability to encode, store, and retrieve information. Different types of memory rely on distinct neural systems. The hippocampus is critical for the formation of new episodic memories (memories of personal experiences) and spatial memories [5]. The amygdala is involved in emotional memories [6]. The cerebellum is important for procedural memory (memory for skills and habits) [7]. The PFC plays a role in working memory (the ability to hold information in mind for a short period) and retrieval of long-term memories [8]. The medial temporal lobe (MTL), consisting of the hippocampus, entorhinal cortex, perirhinal cortex, and parahippocampal cortex, is crucial for declarative memory (memory for facts and events) [9]. Damage to the MTL can result in anterograde amnesia (the inability to form new memories) or retrograde amnesia (the loss of memories from the past).

2.3 Language

Language is a complex cognitive system that allows us to communicate thoughts and ideas through spoken or written symbols. Broca’s area, located in the left inferior frontal gyrus, is essential for speech production [10]. Wernicke’s area, located in the left superior temporal gyrus, is crucial for language comprehension [11]. These two areas are connected by a bundle of nerve fibers called the arcuate fasciculus, which is involved in the integration of speech production and comprehension [12]. The angular gyrus and supramarginal gyrus, located in the parietal lobe, are also involved in language processing, particularly reading and writing [13]. It is worth noting that these regions are highly lateralized to the left hemisphere in most individuals, although right hemisphere contributions exist.

2.4 Executive Function

Executive function encompasses a set of higher-order cognitive processes that allow us to plan, organize, and regulate our behavior. These functions include working memory, cognitive flexibility, inhibitory control, and planning. The PFC is the primary brain region responsible for executive function [14]. Different regions of the PFC contribute to specific executive functions. The dorsolateral PFC (DLPFC) is involved in working memory, cognitive flexibility, and planning [15]. The ventrolateral PFC (VLPFC) is involved in inhibitory control and response selection [16]. The orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) is involved in decision-making and emotional regulation [17].

2.5 Visuospatial Abilities

Visuospatial abilities allow us to perceive, analyze, and manipulate visual information. These abilities are crucial for tasks such as navigation, object recognition, and mental imagery. The parietal lobe, particularly the posterior parietal cortex, is critical for visuospatial processing [18]. The dorsal stream of visual processing, which projects from the visual cortex to the parietal lobe, is involved in spatial perception and action [19]. The ventral stream of visual processing, which projects from the visual cortex to the temporal lobe, is involved in object recognition [20]. Hippocampal formation is also important for spatial navigation and the creation of cognitive maps of the environment [21].

Many thanks to our sponsor Maggie who helped us prepare this research report.

3. Factors Influencing Cognitive Function

Cognitive function is influenced by a complex interplay of genetic, environmental, and lifestyle factors. Understanding these influences is crucial for identifying individuals at risk for cognitive decline and developing strategies to promote cognitive health.

3.1 Genetics

Genetics play a significant role in determining an individual’s cognitive abilities. Studies have shown that cognitive traits, such as intelligence and memory, are highly heritable [22]. Specific genes have been linked to cognitive function, including genes involved in synaptic plasticity, neurotransmitter systems, and brain development [23]. For example, the apolipoprotein E (APOE) gene, which is involved in lipid metabolism, has been strongly associated with Alzheimer’s disease risk and cognitive decline [24]. However, it’s important to note that genetic influences are complex and polygenic, meaning that multiple genes interact with each other and with environmental factors to shape cognitive abilities.

3.2 Age

Age-related cognitive decline is a common phenomenon, characterized by a gradual decline in memory, attention, and executive function [25]. These declines are associated with structural and functional changes in the brain, including a decrease in brain volume, reduced synaptic density, and altered neurotransmitter systems [26]. The PFC and hippocampus are particularly vulnerable to age-related changes, which may contribute to the decline in executive function and memory [27]. However, the rate and severity of cognitive decline vary considerably among individuals, suggesting that other factors, such as lifestyle and health status, also play a role. Furthermore, while some cognitive abilities decline with age, others, such as crystallized intelligence (accumulated knowledge and experience), may remain stable or even improve [28].

3.3 Lifestyle Factors

Lifestyle factors, such as diet, exercise, and social engagement, can have a profound impact on cognitive function. A healthy diet, rich in fruits, vegetables, and omega-3 fatty acids, has been associated with improved cognitive performance and reduced risk of cognitive decline [29]. Regular physical exercise can enhance cerebral blood flow, promote neurogenesis (the formation of new neurons), and improve cognitive function [30]. Social engagement and mentally stimulating activities can also help to maintain cognitive health by promoting neural plasticity and cognitive reserve [31].

Conversely, unhealthy lifestyle choices, such as smoking, excessive alcohol consumption, and a sedentary lifestyle, can negatively impact cognitive function. Smoking damages blood vessels and reduces cerebral blood flow, increasing the risk of cognitive decline and dementia [32]. Heavy alcohol consumption can lead to brain damage and cognitive impairment, even after prolonged abstinence [33]. A sedentary lifestyle is associated with an increased risk of obesity, diabetes, and cardiovascular disease, all of which can negatively impact cognitive function [34].

3.4 Neurological and Psychiatric Conditions

Various neurological and psychiatric conditions can significantly impair cognitive function. Alzheimer’s disease, a progressive neurodegenerative disorder, is the most common cause of dementia, characterized by a gradual decline in memory, language, and executive function [35]. Parkinson’s disease, another neurodegenerative disorder, affects motor control but can also lead to cognitive impairment, particularly in executive function and attention [36]. Stroke, a sudden interruption of blood flow to the brain, can cause focal brain damage and result in a variety of cognitive deficits, depending on the location and extent of the damage [37].

Psychiatric conditions, such as depression and schizophrenia, can also impair cognitive function. Depression is associated with deficits in attention, memory, and executive function [38]. Schizophrenia is characterized by a wide range of cognitive deficits, including deficits in attention, working memory, and executive function [39]. Furthermore, traumatic brain injury (TBI) can lead to a spectrum of cognitive impairments, from mild to severe, depending on the severity and location of the injury [40].

Many thanks to our sponsor Maggie who helped us prepare this research report.

4. Cognitive Plasticity

Cognitive plasticity refers to the brain’s ability to adapt and reorganize in response to experience or injury. This plasticity allows us to learn new skills, adapt to changing environments, and recover from brain damage. Several mechanisms contribute to cognitive plasticity, including synaptic plasticity (changes in the strength of connections between neurons), neurogenesis (the formation of new neurons), and changes in brain structure and function [41].

4.1 Synaptic Plasticity

Synaptic plasticity is a fundamental mechanism of learning and memory. It involves changes in the strength of synaptic connections between neurons, making it easier or harder for neurons to communicate with each other [42]. Long-term potentiation (LTP) is a form of synaptic plasticity that strengthens synaptic connections, while long-term depression (LTD) weakens synaptic connections [43]. These processes are crucial for encoding new memories and adapting to changing environments. It’s becoming increasingly evident that these processes are heavily influenced by neuromodulators, such as dopamine and acetylcholine, further highlighting the complexity of the neural system.

4.2 Neurogenesis

Neurogenesis, the formation of new neurons, occurs throughout life in certain brain regions, particularly the hippocampus [44]. New neurons generated in the hippocampus may play a role in learning and memory, particularly in the formation of new episodic memories [45]. Factors that promote neurogenesis include exercise, enriched environments, and certain medications [46].

4.3 Structural and Functional Changes

The brain can also undergo structural and functional changes in response to experience or injury. For example, learning a new skill can lead to an increase in the size of brain regions involved in that skill [47]. After a stroke or brain injury, the brain can reorganize itself to compensate for the damaged areas, allowing individuals to regain some of their lost cognitive functions [48]. This reorganization may involve recruiting other brain regions to take over the functions of the damaged areas or strengthening existing neural connections to improve efficiency.

Many thanks to our sponsor Maggie who helped us prepare this research report.

5. Cognitive Enhancement

Cognitive enhancement refers to interventions designed to improve cognitive function in healthy individuals or individuals with cognitive impairments. These interventions can range from lifestyle modifications to pharmacological approaches to cognitive training programs.

5.1 Lifestyle Interventions

Lifestyle interventions, such as diet, exercise, and social engagement, can be effective for enhancing cognitive function. A healthy diet, rich in fruits, vegetables, and omega-3 fatty acids, can provide the brain with the nutrients it needs to function optimally [49]. Regular physical exercise can improve cerebral blood flow, promote neurogenesis, and enhance cognitive function [50]. Social engagement and mentally stimulating activities can also help to maintain cognitive health by promoting neural plasticity and cognitive reserve [51].

5.2 Cognitive Training

Cognitive training involves practicing specific cognitive tasks to improve performance in those tasks and potentially transfer those improvements to other cognitive domains. Cognitive training programs have been developed to target a variety of cognitive functions, including attention, memory, and executive function [52]. Some studies have shown that cognitive training can improve cognitive performance in both healthy individuals and individuals with cognitive impairments [53]. However, the effectiveness of cognitive training can vary depending on the type of training, the intensity of training, and the individual’s cognitive abilities.

5.3 Pharmacological Interventions

Pharmacological interventions can also be used to enhance cognitive function. Some medications, such as stimulants and cholinesterase inhibitors, can improve attention, memory, and executive function in individuals with certain cognitive disorders [54]. However, these medications can also have side effects, and their effectiveness can vary depending on the individual. There is also growing interest in the potential for nootropics (cognitive enhancers) to improve cognitive function in healthy individuals, although the evidence for their effectiveness and safety is still limited [55].

5.4 Brain Stimulation Techniques

Non-invasive brain stimulation techniques, such as transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) and transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS), are emerging as potential tools for cognitive enhancement. TMS uses magnetic pulses to stimulate or inhibit activity in specific brain regions, while tDCS uses a weak electrical current to modulate brain activity [56]. These techniques have shown promise for improving cognitive function in a variety of domains, including attention, memory, and executive function [57]. However, further research is needed to optimize these techniques and determine their long-term effects.

Many thanks to our sponsor Maggie who helped us prepare this research report.

6. Future Directions

Despite significant advances in our understanding of cognitive function, many questions remain unanswered. Future research should focus on the following areas:

  • Identifying specific genes and gene networks that contribute to cognitive abilities and cognitive decline. Genome-wide association studies (GWAS) and other genetic approaches can help to identify novel genes and pathways involved in cognitive function.
  • Developing more sensitive and specific biomarkers for early detection of cognitive decline. Biomarkers derived from brain imaging, cerebrospinal fluid, or blood samples could help to identify individuals at risk for cognitive decline before symptoms become apparent.
  • Investigating the mechanisms by which lifestyle factors influence cognitive function. Longitudinal studies and randomized controlled trials can help to determine the causal effects of diet, exercise, and social engagement on cognitive health.
  • Developing more effective cognitive training programs that can transfer to real-world settings. Cognitive training programs should be designed to target specific cognitive deficits and should be tailored to the individual’s cognitive abilities and goals.
  • Exploring the potential of novel pharmacological and non-pharmacological interventions for cognitive enhancement. Further research is needed to evaluate the effectiveness and safety of nootropics, brain stimulation techniques, and other emerging interventions.
  • Investigating individual differences in cognitive resilience and susceptibility to cognitive decline. Understanding why some individuals are more resilient to cognitive decline than others could help to identify protective factors and develop targeted interventions.
  • Employing more ecologically valid cognitive testing paradigms. Much cognitive research is performed using highly controlled and artificial lab-based tasks. Investigating cognitive function in more naturalistic settings (e.g., using virtual reality, or monitoring real-world behavior) would increase the translational value of the research.

Many thanks to our sponsor Maggie who helped us prepare this research report.

7. Conclusion

Cognitive function is a complex and multifaceted construct that is essential for human intelligence and adaptive behavior. Understanding the neural mechanisms, influencing factors, and plasticity of cognitive function is crucial for promoting cognitive health and developing effective interventions to address cognitive impairments. This report has provided a comprehensive overview of cognitive function, highlighting key areas of research and identifying promising avenues for future investigation. By continuing to advance our understanding of cognitive function, we can improve the lives of individuals with cognitive impairments and promote cognitive health throughout the lifespan. Moreover, ethical considerations regarding cognitive enhancement need careful consideration to ensure equitable access and prevent potential misuse.

Many thanks to our sponsor Maggie who helped us prepare this research report.

References

[1] Miller, E. K., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24(1), 167-202.

[2] Corbetta, M., & Shulman, G. L. (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(3), 201-215.

[3] Botvinick, M. M., Cohen, J. D., & Carter, C. S. (2004). Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: an update. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(12), 539-546.

[4] Corbetta, M., Patel, G., & Shulman, G. L. (2008). The reorienting system of the human brain: from environment to theory of mind. Neuron, 58(3), 306-324.

[5] Scoville, W. B., & Milner, B. (1957). Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 20(1), 11-21.

[6] LeDoux, J. E. (2000). Emotion circuits in the brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 23(1), 155-184.

[7] Thompson, R. F. (2005). In search of memory traces. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 28(1), 1-21.

[8] Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory: looking back and looking forward. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4(10), 829-839.

[9] Squire, L. R., Stark, C. E., & Clark, R. E. (2004). The medial temporal lobe. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27(1), 279-306.

[10] Broca, P. (1861). Remarks on the seat of the faculty of articulate language, following an observation of aphemia (loss of speech). Bulletins de la Société d’Anthropologie de Paris, 2(1), 330-357.

[11] Wernicke, C. (1874). Der aphasische Symptomencomplex: Eine psychologische Studie auf anatomischer Basis. Breslau: Weigert.

[12] Catani, M., Jones, D. K., & Ffytche, D. H. (2005). Perisylvian language networks of the human brain. Annals of Neurology, 57(1), 8-16.

[13] Geschwind, N. (1970). The organization of language and the brain. Science, 170(3961), 940-944.

[14] Fuster, J. M. (2001). The prefrontal cortex—an update: time is of the essence. Neuron, 30(2), 319-333.

[15] Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1995). Cellular basis of working memory. Neuron, 14(3), 477-485.

[16] Aron, A. R., Robbins, T. W., & Poldrack, R. A. (2004). Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex: converging evidence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(4), 170-177.

[17] Bechara, A., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. R. (2000). Emotion, decision making and the orbitofrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 10(3), 295-307.

[18] Ungerleider, L. G., & Mishkin, M. (1982). Two cortical visual systems. In D. J. Ingle, M. A. Goodale, & R. J. W. Mansfield (Eds.), Analysis of visual behavior (pp. 549-586). MIT Press.

[19] Goodale, M. A., & Milner, A. D. (1992). Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Trends in Neurosciences, 15(1), 20-25.

[20] Grill-Spector, K., & Malach, R. (2004). The human visual cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27(1), 649-677.

[21] O’Keefe, J., & Nadel, L. (1978). The hippocampus as a cognitive map. Oxford University Press.

[22] Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., Knopik, V. S., & Neiderhiser, J. M. (2016). Behavioral genetics (7th ed.). Worth Publishers.

[23] Deary, I. J., Johnson, W., & Houlihan, L. M. (2009). Genetic foundations of human intelligence. Behavior Genetics, 39(1), 1-18.

[24] Corder, E. H., Saunders, A. M., Strittmatter, W. J., Schmechel, D. E., Gaskell, P. C., Small, G. W., … Roses, A. D. (1993). Gene dose of apolipoprotein E type 4 allele and the risk of Alzheimer’s disease in late onset families. Science, 261(5123), 921-923.

[25] Park, D. C., & Reuter-Lorenz, P. (2009). The adaptive brain: aging and neurocognitive scaffolding. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 173-196.

[26] Raz, N., Gunning-Dixon, F. M., Head, D., Rodrigue, K. M., Williamson, A., & Acker, J. D. (2005). Age and aging effects on the cerebral cortex: quantitative attribute of the in vivo MRI-assessed morphology. Cerebral Cortex, 15(11), 1671-1682.

[27] Small, S. A., Tsai, W. K., DeLaPaz, R., Mayeux, R., & Stern, Y. (2002). Imaging hippocampal function across the human life span: is memory encoding activity preserved?. Annals of Neurology, 51(5), 581-587.

[28] Horn, J. L., & Cattell, R. B. (1967). Age differences in fluid and crystallized intelligence. Acta Psychologica, 26, 107-129.

[29] Gómez-Pinilla, F. (2008). Brain foods: the effects of nutrients on brain function. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(7), 568-578.

[30] Hillman, C. H., Erickson, K. I., & Kramer, A. F. (2008). Be smart, exercise your heart: exercise effects on brain and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(1), 58-65.

[31] Stern, Y. (2002). What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 8(3), 448-460.

[32] Durazzo, T. C., Meyerhoff, D. J., & Nixon, S. J. (2014). Chronic cigarette smoking: Implications for neurocognition and brain neurobiology. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 11(12), 12582–12611.

[33] Crews, F. T., & Nixon, K. (2009). Mechanisms of alcohol-induced brain damage. Alcohol Research & Health, 32(3), 170–180.

[34] Kivipelto, M., Ngandu, T., Laatikainen, T., Winblad, B., Antikainen, R., Enlund, H., … & Soininen, H. (2013). Risk score for the prediction of dementia risk in 20 years among middle aged people: a prospective, population-based study. The Lancet Neurology, 12(8), 735-741.

[35] McKhann, G. M., Knopman, D. S., Chertkow, H., Hyman, B. T., Jack, C. R., Jr, Kawas, C. H., … & Phelps, C. H. (2011). The diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 7(3), 263-269.

[36] Dubois, B., Burn, D., Goetz, C., Aarsland, D., Brown, R. G., Brozova, H., … & Emre, M. (2007). Diagnostic procedures for Parkinson’s disease dementia: Movement Disorder Society Task Force guidelines. Movement Disorders, 22(16), 2314-2324.

[37] Cumming, T. B., Marshall, R. S., Lazar, R. M. (2013). Stroke. In: Bradley W, Daroff R, Fenichel G, Jankovic J, eds. Neurology in Clinical Practice. 6th ed. Philadelphia, PA: Saunders Elsevier.

[38] Rock, P. L., Roiser, J. P., Riedel, W. J., & Blackwell, A. D. (2014). Cognitive impairment in depression: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine, 44(11), 2029-2040.

[39] Green, M. F., Nuechterlein, K. H., Gold, J. M., Barch, D. M., Cohen, J. D., Essock, S. M., … & Velligan, D. I. (2004). Approaching a consensus cognitive battery for clinical trials in schizophrenia: the NIMH-MATRICS conference on assessment of cognition. Biological Psychiatry, 56(5), 301-307.

[40] Menon, D. K., Schwab, K., Wright, D. W., Maas, A. I., & for the Demographics and Clinical Assessment Working Group of the International and Interagency Initiative toward Common Data Elements for Research on Traumatic Brain Injury and Psychological Health. (2010). Position statement: definition of traumatic brain injury. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 91(11), 1637-1640.

[41] Dayan, P., & Cohen, J. D. (2011). Simple rules for complex behaviour. Nature, 469(7330), 377-379.

[42] Bliss, T. V. P., & Lømo, T. (1973). Long lasting potentiation of synaptic transmission in the dentate area of the anaesthetized rabbit following stimulation of the perforant path. The Journal of Physiology, 232(2), 331-356.

[43] Dudek, S. M., & Bear, M. F. (1992). Homosynaptic long-term depression in area CA1 of hippocampus and effects of N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor blockade. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 89(21), 10404-10408.

[44] Eriksson, P. S., Perfilieva, E., Björk-Eriksson, T., Alborn, A. M., Nordborg, C., Peterson, D. A., & Gage, F. H. (1998). Neurogenesis in the adult human hippocampus. Nature Medicine, 4(11), 1313-1317.

[45] Zhao, C., Deng, W., & Gage, F. H. (2008). Mechanisms and functional implications of adult neurogenesis. Cell, 132(4), 645-660.

[46] van Praag, H., Kempermann, G., & Gage, F. H. (1999). Running increases cell proliferation and neurogenesis in the adult mouse hippocampus. Nature Neuroscience, 2(3), 266-270.

[47] Draganski, B., Gaser, C., Busch, V., Schuierer, G., Bogdahn, U., & May, A. (2004). Neuroplasticity: changes in grey matter induced by training. Nature, 427(6972), 311-312.

[48] Dancause, N., & Nudo, R. J. (2011). Shaping plasticity after stroke: a complex puzzle. Stroke, 42(4), 1150-1155.

[49] Gómez-Pinilla, F. (2008). Brain foods: the effects of nutrients on brain function. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(7), 568-578.

[50] Hillman, C. H., Erickson, K. I., & Kramer, A. F. (2008). Be smart, exercise your heart: exercise effects on brain and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(1), 58-65.

[51] Stern, Y. (2012). Cognitive reserve in ageing and Alzheimer’s disease. The Lancet Neurology, 11(11), 1006-1012.

[52] Klingberg, T. (2010). Training and plasticity of working memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(7), 317-324.

[53] Owen, A. M., Hampshire, A., Grahn, J. A., Stenton, R., Dajkovic, N., Burns, A. S., … & Ballard, C. G. (2010). Putting brain training to the test. Nature, 465(7299), 775-778.

[54] Modafinil in Neurology: The Evidence for Cognitive Benefits. (2012). CNS Neuroscience & Therapeutics, 18, 563-564.

[55] Battleday, R. M., & Brem, A. K. (2015). Potential cognitive enhancing effects of coffee caffeine in patients with mild cognitive impairment. British Journal of Nutrition, 114(9), 1461-1468.

[56] Bestmann, S., de Berker, A. O., & Bonstrup, M. (2015). Transcranial magnetic stimulation: from single pulses to patterned stimulation and connectivity. Current Biology, 25(21), R1078-R1088.

[57] Coffman, B. A., Clark, V. P., & Brunel, L. (2014). The role of prefrontal cortex in cognitive enhancement. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 8, 30.

Be the first to comment

Leave a Reply

Your email address will not be published.


*